Afrikaans huistaal die afgelope 100 jaar : die bysin as gevallestudie

Show simple item record

dc.contributor.author Fouché-Karsten, Nadine
dc.contributor.author Wierenga, Roné
dc.contributor.author Breed, Adri
dc.contributor.author Pilon, Suléne
dc.date.accessioned 2025-03-24T13:02:18Z
dc.date.available 2025-03-24T13:02:18Z
dc.date.issued 2025-03
dc.description.abstract Die grense van grammatikale kategorieë wat op skoolvlak gebruik word, soos bysinne, behoort nie wollerig te wees nie. Tog wys Van der Merwe (1966) op die onduidelike grense van bysinkategorieë en op die feit dat Afrikaansonderwysers “skei waar nie geskei behoort te word nie”. Dit wil voorkom of hierdie verwarring steeds bestaan, soos blyk uit ’n meningsopname in 2024 op sosiale media. Omdat bysinne gesofistikeerde sinskonstruksies skep en taalgebruikers bemagtig om hul ervarings van die wêreld uit te druk, wou ons vasstel tot watter mate die bysinkategorieë wat in skoolhandboeke van die afgelope 100 jaar onderskei word, deur wollerigheid gekenmerk word. Hiervoor is ’n bestekopname gedoen, beginnende by Afrikaanse Spraakkuns (Bouman & Pienaar, 1924) tot by die resentste KABV-belynde handboeke vir graad 12. Daar is bevind dat die grense van die bysinkategorieë wat in hierdie bronne onderskei word, inderdaad “wollerig” is. Wollerige gre nse ontstaan, omdat: (i) bysinne wat dieselfde inleier het en logieserwys as dieselfde soort bysin beskou behoort te word, op grond van hul funksie as verskillende soorte bysinne gekategoriseer word; (ii) die kombinasie van verskillende kriteria vir kategorisering gebruik word; en (iii) die grense van verskillende subkategorieë van veral die bywoordelike bysin wollerig is, omdat betekenis as kategoriseringskriterium gebruik word en dieselfde bysin gevolglik as meer as een soort gekategoriseer kan word. Meerduidige kategorisering en ’n skeiding tussen bepalings en bysinne (wat as bepalings gebruik word) is ook aan die orde van die dag. en_US
dc.description.abstract Categorisation is an inherent characteristic of all approaches to grammar. Although the boundaries of grammatical categories at school level should not be fuzzy, it seems that this is in fact the case. Fuzzy boundaries of grammatical categories are problematic within the context of school instruction, as any pedagogical school grammar, according to Bourke (2005:85) and Thornbury (1999:32), should meet specific criteria. Among such criteria are restrictedness and simplicity, which implies that a pedagogical grammar must clearly indicate what the boundaries of a given grammatical unit are. Van der Merwe (1966) refers to, for example, the unclear boundaries of subordinate clauses, stating that teachers of Afrikaans separate where separation should not occur, and this confusion seems to persist, as evidenced by a survey conducted on social media in 2024. Since subordinate clauses create sophisticated sentence constructions and enable language users to express their experiences of the world, it was important to determine to what extent subordinate clauses at school level are characterised by fuzziness. To this end, a survey was conducted, beginning with Afrikaanse Spraakkuns (Bouman & Pienaar, 1924) and extending the survey to the most recent CAPS-aligned textbooks for Grade 12 learners. From the conducted survey it is clear that the boundaries of subordinate clauses at school level have indeed been fuzzy over the past 100 years. Fuzzy boundaries arise because subordinate clauses that are introduced by the same word class and should therefore logically be considered the same type of subordinate clause, are assigned to different types of subordinate clauses, based on their function. Although functional criteria are primarily used to distinguish the main types of subordinate clauses, this criterion is often associated with formal, syntactic, or semantic properties. This combination of different criteria for categorisation also indicates the poorly defined nature of subordinate clauses, or their fuzzy boundaries. Furthermore, the boundaries of various subcategories, particularly adverbial subordinate clauses, are fuzzy, as meaning is most often used as a criterion. This means that the same subordinate clause (e.g., Jason het te veel kersies geëet, [sodat] hy nou maagpyn het) can be classified in multiple ways, either as an adverbial clause of degree (answering the question “To what extent?”) or as an adverbial clause of contingency (answering the question “What happened then?”). Certain gaps regarding clause categorisation were also noted. For instance, in the sentence Die berig dat hy dood is, is onwaar, the subordinate clause dat hy dood is, is regarded as a subject clause, even though the main clause (Die berig is onwaar) has a subject (die berig). This is in contrast with the guideline that a clause can only be an object/ subject clause if such a part of the main clause is not present. Therefore, dat hy dood is should rather be classified as another type of clause. Den Hertog (1904:70) classifies such clauses that fulfil the function of modifiers of nouns, limiting the representation of the noun or providing secondary information about the noun, as adjective clauses. We also noted a shift from viewing the modifier as a syntactic function that can be performed by different grammatical elements (including subordinate clauses), to viewing it as a formal-functional grammatical unit that is limited solely to the simple sentence and contrasted with subordinate clauses. For instance, in Den Hertog (1904:67) it is acknowledged that a subordinate clause can have the function of an adverbial when used as a modifier that restricts the action/state of the verb, therefore rendering it an adverbial clause. Likewise a subordinate clause can fulfil the function of a modifier of a noun within a noun phrase, leading to its classification as an adjective clause (compare Den Hertog, 1904:67). Modifiers have since been understood as a group of words without a predicate that is used for modification and is contrasted with subordinate clauses (compare Vermaak et al., 2013). en_US
dc.description.department Afrikaans en_US
dc.description.librarian hj2024 en_US
dc.description.uri http://ojs.tgwsak.co.za/index.php/TGW/about en_US
dc.identifier.citation Fouche-Karsten, N., Wierenga, R., Breed, A. & Pilon, S. 2025, 'Afrikaans huistaal die afgelope 100 jaar : die bysin as gevallestudie', Tydskrif vir Geesteswetenskappe, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 213-237, doi : 10.17159/2224-7912/2025/v65n1a10. en_US
dc.identifier.issn 0041-4751 (print)
dc.identifier.issn 2224-7912 (online)
dc.identifier.other 10.17159/2224-7912/2025/v65n1a10
dc.identifier.uri http://hdl.handle.net/2263/101662
dc.language.iso Afrikaans en_US
dc.publisher Suid Afrikaanse Akademie vir Wetenskap en Kuns en_US
dc.rights This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License. en_US
dc.subject Afrikaans huistaal en_US
dc.subject Bestekopname en_US
dc.subject Byvoeglike bysin en_US
dc.subject Bywoordelike bysin en_US
dc.subject Bysin en_US
dc.subject Gesegdesin en_US
dc.subject Kategorisering en_US
dc.subject Onderwerpsin en_US
dc.subject Skoolhandboeke en_US
dc.subject Voorwerpsin en_US
dc.subject Wollerige grense en_US
dc.subject Adjective clause en_US
dc.subject Adverbial clause en_US
dc.subject Afrikaans home language en_US
dc.subject Categorisation en_US
dc.subject Fuzziness en_US
dc.subject Object clause en_US
dc.subject School textbooks en_US
dc.subject Subordinate clause en_US
dc.subject Subject clause en_US
dc.subject Subject complement clause en_US
dc.subject Surveys en_US
dc.subject.other Humanities articles SDG-04
dc.subject.other SDG-04: Quality education
dc.title Afrikaans huistaal die afgelope 100 jaar : die bysin as gevallestudie en_US
dc.title.alternative Afrikaans home language over the past 100 years : the subordinate clause as a case study en_US
dc.type Article en_US


Files in this item

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record